Home/Nonfiction/Beyond Words
Loading...
Beyond Words cover

Beyond Words

What Animals Think and Feel

4.4 (4,353 ratings)
29 minutes read | Text | 9 key ideas
Witness the soulful heartbeat of the animal kingdom in "Beyond Words." In the shimmering heat of Kenya's Amboseli National Park, elephant families navigate the perils of poaching and drought with remarkable resilience. Meanwhile, the haunting valleys of Yellowstone echo with the cries of wolves confronting the void left by their lost kin. In the tranquil depths of the Pacific Northwest, killer whales reveal a society built on peace and intricate social bonds. This book invites you to contemplate the depth of thought and emotion that animates these creatures, unveiling a tapestry of life as rich and complex as our own. Here, the barriers between human and animal blur, illuminating a shared capacity for emotion and learning that binds all sentient beings.

Categories

Nonfiction, Psychology, Science, Animals, Nature, Audiobook, Africa, Biology, Environment, Ecology

Content Type

Book

Binding

Hardcover

Year

2014

Publisher

Henry Holt & Company Inc

Language

English

ASIN

0805098887

ISBN

0805098887

ISBN13

9780805098884

File Download

PDF | EPUB

Beyond Words Plot Summary

Introduction

When a crow fashions a hook from a piece of wire to retrieve food from a tube, or when an elephant gently touches the bones of a deceased family member, we glimpse something profound about the mental lives of animals. For centuries, humans have wondered what goes on in the minds of other creatures. Do they think? Do they feel? Are they conscious in ways similar to us? These questions have moved from the realm of philosophy into rigorous scientific inquiry, revealing surprising answers that challenge our understanding of cognition itself. The scientific exploration of animal minds has undergone a revolution in recent decades. What was once dismissed as anthropomorphism—the inappropriate attribution of human qualities to animals—has given way to careful experimental evidence revealing remarkable cognitive abilities across the animal kingdom. From self-awareness in elephants and dolphins to cultural traditions in chimpanzees and whales, from complex problem-solving in corvids to emotional intelligence in rats, we now know that many animals possess rich mental lives that parallel our own in surprising ways. This book takes you on a journey through the fascinating world of animal cognition, exploring how different species perceive, think, feel, and relate to one another, ultimately revealing not just how animals think, but how understanding their minds transforms our view of consciousness itself.

Chapter 1: Self-Awareness: Mirror Tests and Recognition Across Species

Self-awareness represents one of the most fascinating frontiers in animal cognition research. The classic test for this ability is the mirror self-recognition test, developed by psychologist Gordon Gallup Jr. in 1970. In this experiment, researchers place a colored mark on an animal in a location they cannot see without a mirror, such as their forehead. If the animal uses the mirror to investigate the mark—touching or examining it—they demonstrate an understanding that the reflection represents themselves rather than another animal. This seemingly simple test reveals a profound cognitive capacity: the ability to recognize oneself as an individual entity. For decades, only humans and great apes passed this test consistently. Chimpanzees, bonobos, and orangutans all demonstrate self-recognition, often using mirrors to examine parts of their bodies they cannot otherwise see. However, the club of self-aware species has expanded remarkably in recent years. Dolphins will spend time examining marked parts of their bodies when presented with mirrors. Asian elephants use their trunks to touch marks placed on their heads when looking at their reflections. Perhaps most surprisingly, magpies—birds with brains structured entirely differently from mammals—will attempt to remove colored stickers placed on their bodies when they can see themselves in mirrors. These findings suggest that self-awareness has evolved independently multiple times across the animal kingdom. The implications of self-recognition extend far beyond a simple party trick. Self-awareness appears to correlate with other sophisticated cognitive abilities, including empathy, perspective-taking, and metacognition (thinking about one's own thoughts). Animals that recognize themselves typically excel in social intelligence, suggesting that understanding oneself may be a prerequisite for understanding others. When an elephant recognizes herself in a mirror, she demonstrates not just visual self-recognition but a concept of "self" that exists separately from others—a fundamental building block for complex social cognition. Critics of the mirror test point out its limitations. The test relies heavily on visual processing and may not be appropriate for species that navigate their world primarily through other senses. Dogs, for instance, recognize their own scent but generally fail mirror tests, suggesting their self-awareness may operate through olfaction rather than vision. Cultural factors also play a role—human children from cultures without mirrors initially fail the test despite possessing self-awareness. These critiques don't invalidate the test but suggest self-awareness may be more widespread than mirror tests alone can reveal. What makes self-awareness particularly fascinating is its discontinuous distribution across species. It doesn't simply correlate with brain size or evolutionary relatedness to humans. Magpies recognize themselves while many primates do not. Elephants pass the test while their close relatives, rhinos and hippos, apparently fail. This pattern suggests that self-awareness evolves in response to specific ecological and social pressures rather than representing a single evolutionary "advance." Each self-aware species has developed this capacity independently, tailored to its unique environmental challenges and social structures. The study of self-awareness in animals ultimately challenges our understanding of consciousness itself. If consciousness exists on a spectrum rather than as a binary quality, then the gap between human and animal minds narrows considerably. The elephant who recognizes herself in a mirror may experience a form of subjective awareness not so different from our own, inviting us to reconsider our place in the cognitive landscape of life on Earth.

Chapter 2: Emotional Intelligence: How Animals Process Feelings

Emotions were once considered uniquely human experiences, too subjective and complex for "simpler" animal minds. However, scientific evidence now overwhelmingly indicates that many animals experience rich emotional lives that parallel our own in remarkable ways. The foundations for this emotional capacity lie in shared brain structures—the limbic system, which processes emotions like fear, pleasure, and attachment, appears with remarkable consistency across mammalian species. Even the neurotransmitters and hormones that regulate mood—dopamine, serotonin, oxytocin—function virtually identically across diverse animal groups. These shared neural mechanisms produce recognizable emotional responses across species. When a dog wags its tail upon seeing its owner, or when an elephant trumpets and runs to greet a long-absent family member, we witness joy expressed through species-specific behaviors but stemming from similar emotional processes. Fear responses show even greater consistency—the freeze-flight-fight cascade appears across vertebrates from fish to mammals, suggesting ancient evolutionary origins for this fundamental emotion. What differs between species isn't the capacity for basic emotions but the cognitive complexity surrounding them. A mouse feels fear, but a chimpanzee might additionally contemplate the source of danger and remember past encounters. Grief provides particularly compelling evidence for complex emotions in animals. Elephants show remarkable responses to death, including standing vigil over deceased family members, gently touching and examining the remains with their trunks, and returning to the bones years later. Chimpanzees have been observed carrying dead infants for days or weeks, grooming and protecting the bodies despite their decomposition. These behaviors suggest not just momentary distress but a deeper understanding of death and loss. When a mother orca carried her dead calf for 17 days across 1,000 miles in 2018, researchers witnessed what appeared to be profound grief expressed through persistent, purposeful behavior. Social emotions like empathy and compassion extend beyond primates and elephants. Rats will free trapped companions even when there's no reward for doing so—and even when it means sharing a valued food resource. When one rat observes another in distress, its stress hormones rise in response, demonstrating emotional contagion—a basic form of empathy. This capacity for emotional resonance serves crucial functions in social species, facilitating cooperation, caregiving, and group cohesion. Far from being luxury features, emotions provide essential information about environmental threats, opportunities, and social dynamics. The scientific study of animal emotions faces unique challenges. Unlike humans, animals cannot verbally report their subjective experiences. Researchers must therefore rely on behavioral indicators, physiological measurements, and careful experimental designs that control for alternative explanations. The field has developed rigorous methods to distinguish genuine emotions from simpler processes, including looking for consistent patterns across multiple indicators and testing whether animals will work to seek or avoid particular emotional states. Understanding animal emotions transforms both science and ethics. If animals experience joy, grief, fear, and attachment in ways comparable to humans, their welfare matters morally in ways that purely mechanical entities do not. This recognition has already begun reshaping practices from laboratory research to agriculture, with growing emphasis on providing environments that support positive emotional states in animals. Beyond practical implications, recognizing animal emotions challenges the traditional boundary between humans and other species, revealing emotional lives that exist on a continuum rather than in separate categories.

Chapter 3: Social Cognition: The Complex Web of Animal Relationships

Social cognition—the ability to understand, predict, and respond to others' behavior—represents one of the most sophisticated forms of intelligence in the animal kingdom. At its foundation lies the capacity to recognize individuals and remember their behavior over time. Many species demonstrate remarkable abilities in this domain. Elephants can recognize and remember hundreds of other elephants, distinguishing between friends, family members, and strangers through sight, sound, and scent. Corvids like ravens and crows remember individuals who have treated them well or poorly, adjusting their behavior accordingly even years later. This individual recognition forms the foundation for complex social relationships that extend far beyond simple group living. The most sophisticated social cognition involves what scientists call "theory of mind"—understanding that others have thoughts, beliefs, and intentions that may differ from one's own. Chimpanzees show evidence of this capacity in various contexts. They modify their food-seeking behavior based on whether a dominant individual can see them, suggesting they understand the concept of visual perspective. In experimental settings, they preferentially help humans who appear unaware of where food is hidden rather than those who already know, indicating an understanding of others' knowledge states. While animal theory of mind may not reach the recursive complexity of human social reasoning ("she thinks that he believes that I want"), the foundations appear present in many species. Social knowledge in many species extends beyond dyadic relationships to include understanding of third-party interactions—who is friends with whom, who outranks whom, who is related to whom. Baboons respond with surprise when they hear vocalizations suggesting that dominance relationships have been violated, indicating they maintain mental representations of the social hierarchy. Dolphins form nested alliances where groups of males cooperate to compete with other groups, requiring sophisticated tracking of multiple relationships simultaneously. This social knowledge isn't innate but learned through observation and experience, constituting a form of social intelligence that develops over the lifespan. Communication plays a crucial role in social cognition. Many species use sophisticated communication systems to coordinate group activities, warn of dangers, and maintain relationships. Vervet monkeys use distinct alarm calls for different predators—snakes, eagles, or leopards—each triggering appropriate escape responses. Prairie dogs employ a complex system of calls that specify not just the presence of a predator but its size, shape, color, and speed of movement. These communication systems, while not equivalent to human language, demonstrate remarkable specificity and functionality within their ecological contexts. The evolution of social cognition appears driven by particular ecological and social pressures. Species that form stable social groups, face complex foraging challenges, or engage in cooperative breeding typically show more sophisticated social intelligence. This pattern supports the "social brain hypothesis," which proposes that managing complex social relationships was a primary driver for the evolution of larger brains and advanced cognition. The most socially complex species—great apes, elephants, cetaceans, and certain birds—also tend to show the most sophisticated cognition across multiple domains. Understanding animal social cognition challenges traditional views of intelligence as a single, linear scale with humans at the apex. Different species have evolved distinct forms of social intelligence adapted to their particular ecological niches and social structures. A wolf coordinating with packmates during a hunt, an elephant matriarch making decisions based on decades of social knowledge, and a raven engaging in tactical deception all demonstrate sophisticated social cognition, even if these abilities manifest differently from human social intelligence. This diversity of social minds reveals cognition not as a ladder with humans at the top, but as a bush with multiple branches of intelligence evolving in parallel across the animal kingdom.

Chapter 4: Tool Use and Problem Solving: Intelligence in Action

Tool use, once considered a uniquely human ability, has now been documented across a remarkable diversity of species, revealing sophisticated problem-solving capacities that challenge traditional views of animal intelligence. The most extensive tool users outside humans are chimpanzees, who employ a variety of tools for different purposes. They select appropriate branches to fish for termites, carefully modifying them by stripping leaves and chewing the ends to create effective tools. Some populations use stone hammers and anvils to crack nuts, with young chimpanzees spending years observing adults before mastering the technique. These tool traditions vary between communities, creating distinct "cultures" of tool use that are transmitted socially rather than genetically. Birds, particularly corvids (crows, ravens, and their relatives), demonstrate astonishing tool abilities despite having brains structured entirely differently from mammals. New Caledonian crows craft hooked tools from twigs to extract insects from crevices, and can solve multi-step problems requiring remarkable causal understanding. In laboratory settings, these crows have demonstrated the ability to use one tool to retrieve another tool, which is then used to obtain food—a form of sequential tool use previously thought limited to great apes. Perhaps most impressively, when presented with a straight piece of wire and food in a tube, some crows spontaneously bend the wire into a hook to retrieve the food, showing an understanding of the required tool properties without prior training. What makes these examples particularly significant is that they often involve insight rather than trial-and-error learning. When faced with novel problems, many tool-using species demonstrate "aha moments"—sudden realizations about potential solutions. This suggests they're not just associating actions with rewards but mentally representing problems and potential solutions. A capuchin monkey encountering a tube with food visible but out of reach might immediately seek water to raise the floating food, rather than trying random actions. This kind of causal understanding represents a sophisticated form of intelligence that allows animals to solve novel problems they've never encountered before. Problem-solving extends beyond tool use to include various forms of environmental manipulation. Elephants have been observed moving boxes to stand on to reach hanging food, understanding the concept of creating elevated platforms. Orangutans in captivity have used water as a tool to raise floating objects within reach. Even octopuses, with nervous systems radically different from vertebrates, demonstrate remarkable problem-solving abilities—unscrewing jar lids, navigating mazes, and even escaping from sealed tanks through tiny openings. These examples suggest that advanced problem-solving has evolved independently multiple times across animal lineages with very different brain structures. The cognitive abilities underlying tool use include several sophisticated capacities: causal understanding (how objects interact), means-end reasoning (using intermediate steps to achieve goals), mental representation (holding images of tools and problems in mind), and sometimes future planning (creating tools in advance of need). These capacities were once considered hallmarks of human cognition, but their presence across diverse species suggests they represent adaptations to particular ecological challenges rather than uniquely human traits. Species that face extractive foraging challenges—obtaining food from hard-to-access places—often evolve tool use independently. Understanding animal problem-solving and tool use forces us to reconsider intelligence as existing on a continuum rather than being uniquely human. The gap between human technology and animal tool use is one of degree rather than kind—a difference in complexity rather than fundamental cognitive capacity. When a crow fashions a hook from wire or an elephant moves a box to stand on, they demonstrate the same basic cognitive processes that underlie human technological innovation, albeit at a simpler level. This perspective places human intelligence within an evolutionary context, revealing our cognitive abilities as elaborate extensions of capacities shared with many other species.

Chapter 5: Communication Systems: Languages Without Words

Animal communication extends far beyond simple calls and displays, revealing sophisticated systems that sometimes approach language-like properties. While no non-human animal possesses full human language with its infinite generativity and complex grammar, many species demonstrate communication abilities that challenge our understanding of what constitutes language. These systems allow animals to convey specific information about food, predators, social relationships, and even abstract concepts, facilitating coordination and cooperation within social groups. Vervet monkeys provide a classic example of referential communication—signals that refer to specific objects or events in the environment. They produce distinct alarm calls for different predator types: one for eagles, another for leopards, and a third for snakes. Each call triggers appropriate escape responses—running into bushes for eagles, climbing trees for leopards, and standing upright to spot snakes. Experiments playing recorded calls elicit these specific responses even when no predator is present, demonstrating that the calls themselves carry meaning. This system represents a primitive form of semantics, where specific sounds consistently refer to specific external threats. Dolphins communicate through a complex system of whistles, clicks, and body movements. Each dolphin develops a unique "signature whistle" that functions essentially as a name. When separated from their pod, dolphins repeatedly broadcast their signature whistle, allowing others to identify and locate them. More remarkably, dolphins will sometimes produce the signature whistles of absent companions, apparently "calling" specific individuals. This capacity for referential communication about specific individuals represents a sophisticated form of social signaling that facilitates coordination in their dynamic social groups. Honeybees demonstrate perhaps the most complex symbolic communication system outside humans. Through their famous "waggle dance," foragers communicate the distance, direction, and quality of food sources to hivemates. The angle of the dance relative to gravity indicates the direction relative to the sun, while the duration of the waggle run correlates precisely with distance. This abstract symbolic system allows bees to communicate about locations they've never visited, based solely on information from the dance. What makes this system particularly remarkable is its displacement—the ability to communicate about things distant in space and time, a feature once considered unique to human language. Prairie dogs employ an intricate alarm call system that specifies not just the presence of a predator but detailed information about its characteristics. Research by Con Slobodchikoff has revealed that their calls encode information about the predator's species, size, shape, color, and even speed of movement. When researchers presented prairie dogs with novel objects like differently colored shirts, the animals generated new alarm calls specific to these objects, suggesting their communication system can incorporate new referents rather than being fixed and innate. What distinguishes these communication systems from human language? Several key features: Most animal communication lacks the infinite generativity of human language—the ability to create endless novel sentences from finite elements. Animal systems typically have limited vocabulary compared to human languages. And critically, most animal communication lacks complex syntax and grammar that allows humans to express abstract concepts, hypotheticals, and time displacements. These differences represent matters of degree rather than kind, suggesting communication exists on a continuum rather than in discrete categories of "language" versus "non-language." Understanding animal communication challenges us to reconsider language not as a binary trait that humans exclusively possess, but as a spectrum of abilities that evolved to different degrees across species. The question becomes not whether animals have language, but rather which elements of language-like communication they possess, and how these elements evolved to serve their specific ecological and social needs. This perspective places human language within an evolutionary context, revealing it as an elaborate extension of communicative capacities that exist in simpler forms throughout the animal kingdom.

Chapter 6: Cultural Transmission: How Animals Pass Knowledge

Culture—the transmission of knowledge, behaviors, and traditions through social learning rather than genetics—was once considered uniquely human. However, research across diverse species reveals that many animals possess cultural traditions that are learned, maintained, and transmitted across generations, creating distinct "ways of life" within species. These cultural systems allow rapid adaptation to environmental challenges without genetic change and enable the accumulation of knowledge across generations. Chimpanzees provide the most extensively documented example of animal culture. Different chimpanzee communities across Africa have unique behavioral traditions that aren't explained by ecological differences. Some groups crack nuts with stones while others don't, despite having access to the same materials. Some fish for termites by inserting sticks into mounds, while others don't. Researchers have documented over 40 distinct cultural behaviors across chimpanzee communities, including tool use, grooming techniques, and courtship displays. Young chimpanzees learn these traditions through observation and practice, maintaining cultural differences across generations. When researchers track the spread of new behaviors, such as leaf-sponging to collect water, they observe patterns of transmission that mirror human cultural diffusion. Cetaceans (whales and dolphins) demonstrate sophisticated cultural transmission. Killer whale pods develop distinct "dialects" of calls that serve as cultural markers and persist across generations. Different populations specialize in unique hunting techniques—some beach themselves temporarily to catch seals on shore, others create waves to wash seals off ice floes. These techniques are learned, not instinctual, as evidenced by the fact that young whales require years of observation before mastering them. Humpback whales show cultural evolution of songs, with new variations spreading across populations in patterns resembling human fashion trends. When researchers track these song changes, they observe them spreading from west to east across the Pacific Ocean over several years, with each population adopting and modifying the new patterns. Birds, particularly corvids, maintain cultural traditions through social learning. New Caledonian crows craft complex tools with regional variations in design that persist across generations. Japanese carrion crows in urban environments have learned to place nuts on roadways for cars to crack, then retrieve the contents during red lights—a behavior that spread culturally through crow populations. Some bird species even maintain vocal dialects that serve as cultural markers of group identity, with young birds learning the specific "accent" of their natal group. The mechanisms of cultural transmission vary across species but share fundamental similarities. Observational learning allows naive individuals to acquire behaviors by watching experienced performers. In some cases, this learning is facilitated by active teaching, where knowledgeable individuals modify their behavior to facilitate learning in novices. Elephants have been observed guiding younger family members' trunks to show them how to manipulate objects. Meerkats gradually introduce live prey to pups, starting with dead or disabled prey and progressing to more challenging hunting opportunities as the pups develop skills. Cultural transmission offers significant adaptive advantages. It allows rapid behavioral adaptation to new challenges without genetic change. It enables the accumulation of knowledge across generations, creating a form of collective intelligence greater than any individual could achieve alone. And it allows specialization in complex skills that would be impossible to develop within a single lifetime. These advantages explain why culture has evolved independently across diverse lineages, from primates to cetaceans to birds, despite the significant cognitive demands it places on learners and demonstrators. Understanding animal culture forces us to reconsider what makes humans distinct. Rather than possessing unique capacities for cultural transmission, humans may simply have developed more extensive and cumulative cultural systems—a difference of degree rather than kind. The presence of culture across diverse species suggests that social learning represents a fundamental adaptation for complex social living, one that has evolved repeatedly across the animal kingdom. This perspective places human cultural achievements within an evolutionary continuum, revealing them as elaborate extensions of processes that exist in simpler forms throughout the animal world.

Chapter 7: Conservation Implications: Protecting Thinking Species

The growing scientific understanding of animal cognition carries profound implications for conservation efforts worldwide. As we recognize the complex mental lives of other species, traditional approaches to wildlife management and conservation are being transformed by this deeper appreciation of animal intelligence, emotional capacity, and social complexity. Conservation is no longer merely about preserving genes or ecosystems, but about protecting minds—the accumulated knowledge, traditions, and social structures that constitute the cognitive lives of other species. For social species with advanced cognition, conservation must extend beyond simple population numbers to consider social structures and knowledge transmission. Elephant matriarchs carry decades of ecological knowledge—where to find water during droughts, how to navigate to distant feeding grounds, how to respond to predators. When poaching removes these elder females, entire herds lose this vital knowledge. Similarly, killer whale pods maintain unique hunting techniques and vocal traditions that constitute their cultural identity. The loss of key individuals disrupts not just population numbers but the transmission of crucial survival knowledge. This understanding has led to conservation approaches that specifically protect elder individuals and maintain intact social units rather than focusing solely on total population size. Habitat fragmentation takes on new significance when viewed through the lens of animal cognition. For species that maintain cultural traditions tied to specific locations, preserving not just habitat but particular sites of cultural significance becomes crucial. Chimpanzees maintain "tool sites" where generations have learned to crack nuts or fish for termites. Dolphins develop specialized feeding techniques adapted to specific coastal features. Protecting these sites preserves not just physical habitat but cultural heritage. This perspective has led to the concept of "cultural keystones"—specific locations that hold disproportionate importance for the maintenance of animal traditions and knowledge. Trauma and psychological distress increasingly factor into conservation planning. Elephants that have witnessed poaching or culling show symptoms resembling post-traumatic stress disorder in humans—heightened aggression, abnormal social behavior, and elevated stress hormones. Young elephants orphaned by poaching develop abnormally without proper social learning and emotional support. Effective conservation must address these psychological dimensions, sometimes requiring rehabilitation programs that provide surrogate social structures for traumatized animals. The field of conservation psychology has emerged to address these dimensions of animal welfare in wild populations. Conservation strategies themselves are being redesigned based on cognitive understanding. Recognizing that elephants communicate about danger through infrasound has led to the development of "beehive fences" that deter crop-raiding by triggering elephants' learned fear of bees. Understanding that predators can learn to associate certain areas with danger has improved non-lethal deterrent systems. Knowledge of animal communication systems has enhanced monitoring techniques, allowing researchers to "eavesdrop" on populations to assess their status. These approaches represent a shift from treating animals as objects to be managed toward engaging with them as thinking beings with their own perspectives and learning capacities. Perhaps most fundamentally, research on animal cognition challenges the traditional separation between humans and other species that has often justified environmental exploitation. As we recognize cognitive continuity between humans and other animals—shared capacities for thought, feeling, and culture—the philosophical foundation for unlimited human dominance over nature weakens. Conservation becomes reimagined not as humans managing resources but as coexisting with other thinking beings with whom we share evolutionary history and cognitive heritage. This ethical dimension increasingly influences policy decisions about hunting, captivity, and habitat protection, creating a conservation ethic grounded in respect for animal minds rather than merely utilitarian resource management.

Summary

The scientific exploration of animal minds reveals a profound truth: intelligence, consciousness, and emotional complexity exist on a continuum across species rather than being uniquely human traits. From elephants that recognize themselves in mirrors and mourn their dead, to dolphins that call each other by name, to crows that craft tools with insight and foresight, the evidence consistently shows that many animals possess rich inner lives that parallel our own in surprising ways. These cognitive abilities evolved not as stepping stones toward human intelligence, but as adaptations to each species' ecological and social challenges, creating diverse forms of mind across the animal kingdom. This understanding transforms our relationship with the natural world in fundamental ways. It challenges us to reconsider our ethical responsibilities toward other thinking beings, to design conservation strategies that protect not just bodies but minds, and to appreciate the remarkable diversity of consciousness that surrounds us. As we continue to explore the cognitive lives beyond human experience, perhaps the most important question becomes not how animal minds differ from our own, but what responsibilities we bear toward our fellow travelers on the evolutionary journey. For students curious to explore further, this field offers rich opportunities to investigate specific cognitive abilities across species, the evolutionary origins of consciousness, and the ethical implications of recognizing other minds. The study of animal cognition ultimately reveals not just how animals think, but how understanding their minds transforms our view of consciousness itself.

Best Quote

“Why do human egos seem so threatened by the thought that other animals think and feel? Is it because acknowledging the mind of another makes it harder to abuse them?” ― Carl Safina, Beyond Words: What Animals Think and Feel

Review Summary

Strengths: The review highlights the author's credibility as a respected ecologist and conservationist, providing a reliable perspective on animal societies. The book offers in-depth exploration of elephants, wolves, and killer whales, enriched by the author's collaboration with field scientists. It also includes diverse examples from other species like chimps, bonobos, birds, reptiles, and insects, enhancing the thematic depth.\nWeaknesses: The review notes that animal behavior and brain science are not the author's areas of expertise, which might limit the depth in those specific aspects.\nOverall Sentiment: Enthusiastic\nKey Takeaway: The book is a compelling exploration of animal intelligence and emotions, advocating for greater respect and stewardship of animals by humans. It effectively combines scientific insights with the author's ecological expertise to argue for the recognition of animals as sentient beings with complex societies.

About Author

Loading...
Carl Safina Avatar

Carl Safina

Carl Safina’s work has been recognized with MacArthur, Pew, and Guggenheim Fellowships, and his writing has won Orion, Lannan, and National Academies literary awards and the John Burroughs, James Beard, and George Rabb medals. He has a PhD in ecology from Rutgers University. Safina is the inaugural holder of the endowed chair for nature and humanity at Stony Brook University, where he co-chairs the steering committee of the Alan Alda Center for Communicating Science and is founding president of the not-for-profit organization, The Safina Center. He hosted the 10-part PBS series Saving the Ocean with Carl Safina. His writing appears in The New York Times, Audubon, Orion, and other periodicals and on the Web at National Geographic News and Views, Huffington Post, and CNN.com.He lives on Long Island, New York with his wife Patricia, the two best beach-running dogs in the world, some chickens, a couple of parrots, and Frankie the kingsnake.

Read more

Download PDF & EPUB

To save this Black List summary for later, download the free PDF and EPUB. You can print it out, or read offline at your convenience.

Book Cover

Beyond Words

By Carl Safina

0:00/0:00

Build Your Library

Select titles that spark your interest. We'll find bite-sized summaries you'll love.