Home/Nonfiction/Felt Time
Loading...
Felt Time cover

Felt Time

The Psychology of How We Perceive Time

3.5 (573 ratings)
22 minutes read | Text | 9 key ideas
Ever wondered why the minutes stretch in boredom, yet the years vanish in a blink as you age? Marc Wittmann's "Felt Time" unlocks the enigma of our temporal perception, weaving together threads from psychology and neuroscience. As children grow restless with anticipation and adults lament time's acceleration, Wittmann offers insights into the very essence of time and consciousness. Why do some chase instant gratification while others savor the present? How does one's heartbeat govern this silent clock within us? With each page, discover how mindfulness can slow life's relentless pace and turn fleeting moments into a tapestry of fulfilled existence. Wittmann's exploration reveals the secret rhythm of our lives, challenging readers to understand—and perhaps redefine—their own sense of time.

Categories

Nonfiction, Psychology, Philosophy, Science, Education, Audiobook, Physics, Popular Science, Biology, Neuroscience

Content Type

Book

Binding

Kindle Edition

Year

2016

Publisher

The MIT Press

Language

English

ASIN

B01BO1LKPC

File Download

PDF | EPUB

Felt Time Plot Summary

Introduction

Time is a fascinating and elusive concept that affects every aspect of our lives. From the fleeting moments of joy to the seemingly endless periods of waiting, our perception of time shapes our experiences in profound ways. Have you ever noticed how time seems to fly when you're having fun, yet crawls when you're bored or anxious? This peculiar subjective experience of time is at the heart of this exploration into the psychology of temporal perception. Recent scientific discoveries have yielded remarkable insights into how our brains process time. This book delves into the mechanisms behind our sense of temporal duration, revealing how our emotions, bodily states, and attention all influence how we experience the passage of time. You'll discover why childhood summers seemed to last forever while years speed by as we age, how our bodies serve as internal clocks, and why mindfulness practices can alter our perception of the present moment. By understanding the psychology of temporal perception, we gain not only scientific knowledge but also valuable wisdom about living more fulfilling lives in harmony with our internal sense of time.

Chapter 1: The Nature of Temporal Perception

Time perception is fundamentally subjective, varying dramatically between individuals and situations. Unlike our physical senses such as sight or hearing, we don't have a dedicated sensory organ for time. Instead, our experience of time emerges from complex interactions between various brain systems, bodily processes, and psychological states. One of the most fascinating aspects of temporal perception is our ability to delay gratification. Studies like the famous "Marshmallow Test" conducted by Walter Mischel demonstrate this phenomenon clearly. Children were given a choice: eat one marshmallow immediately or wait a short period to receive two marshmallows. What's remarkable is that children who showed greater ability to wait at ages four or five demonstrated better academic performance and social competence ten years later. This reveals how our relationship with time can shape life outcomes. Our perception of time is also affected by what researchers call "temporal myopia" - our tendency to value immediate rewards over larger future benefits. When faced with receiving $1 now or $50 in a week, most people rationally choose to wait. However, when the immediate option increases to $20 or $30, many switch to the smaller immediate reward despite the larger delayed option still being $50. This pattern of temporal discounting follows a hyperbolic curve - the value of future rewards decreases more steeply for near-future delays than for more distant ones. Different cultures exhibit varying temporal orientations as well. Philip Zimbardo's research on time perspectives shows that some societies are more present-oriented while others focus more on the future. Present-oriented individuals tend to take more risks and seek immediate gratification, while future-oriented people are more willing to delay rewards for longer-term goals. Neither perspective is inherently superior - both present-focus and future-planning have their place in a balanced life. This relationship with time isn't fixed - it evolves throughout our lives. Children typically struggle more with waiting compared to adults, as their sense of time and capacity for self-regulation are still developing. Interestingly, individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder often display heightened temporal myopia, suggesting connections between time perception, attention, and impulse control that neuroscientists are just beginning to understand.

Chapter 2: Waiting and Temporal Shortsightedness

When do we become most aware of time? Typically, it's when we're waiting. Whether standing in line at a store, sitting in traffic, or anticipating an important event, waiting makes us acutely conscious of time's passage. This awareness often functions as an "error signal," indicating something isn't proceeding according to our expectations. Our capacity to assess time varies significantly across different durations. For periods lasting up to about three seconds, humans demonstrate remarkable precision in temporal judgment. Beyond this window, our estimations become increasingly variable and less accurate. This suggests our brains process time differently depending on the scale involved - from milliseconds to seconds to minutes and beyond. Scientists have long searched for an internal clock mechanism to explain how we track time. Michel Treisman proposed an influential model in 1963 featuring a "pacemaker" emitting regular pulses collected by an internal counter. When we focus attention on time, more pulses are registered, making durations feel longer. When distracted, fewer pulses are counted, and time seems to pass quickly. This model helps explain why time drags when we're bored and flies when we're engaged - attention plays a crucial role in our temporal experience. Physiological states also dramatically affect our perception of time. Elevated body temperature, whether from fever or emotional arousal, tends to make time feel extended. Hudson Hoagland first observed this phenomenon in the 1930s when his feverish wife consistently overestimated time durations. From an evolutionary perspective, this makes sense - in threatening situations, processing environmental information more rapidly (experiencing a subjective "slow-motion effect") could enhance survival chances. Cultural differences in time perception are profound and well-documented. Robert Levine's research reveals stark contrasts between "event-time" and "clock-time" cultures. In event-time societies, activities determine scheduling - meetings end when business concludes, not when a predetermined time arrives. Clock-time cultures, typical of industrialized nations, organize activities around abstract timeframes. While clock-time cultures may demonstrate greater economic efficiency, event-time cultures often report higher life satisfaction and less time-related stress. This variability in how we experience waiting and time reflects fundamental aspects of human psychology - our attention, emotions, cultural conditioning, and even biology all shape our temporal landscape. Understanding these factors can help us develop healthier relationships with time and waiting in our daily lives.

Chapter 3: Brain Rhythms and Time Processing

Does each person possess their own internal tempo - a personal brain rhythm that distinguishes quick thinkers from slower ones? This question has fascinated scientists studying time perception for decades. Sten Nadolny's novel "The Discovery of Slowness" explores this idea through its protagonist John Franklin, who perceives the world at a markedly slower pace than others. While the novel is fiction, research suggests individual differences in temporal processing do exist. Cognitive psychologists measure these differences using tasks that test temporal order thresholds - the minimum time needed to correctly identify which of two stimuli occurred first. In young adults, these thresholds typically range between 20 and 60 milliseconds, regardless of whether the stimuli are visual, auditory, or tactile. This consistency across sensory modalities suggests a central brain mechanism for processing temporal sequences. Ernst Pöppel, a pioneering researcher in this field, hypothesizes that the brain operates with a fundamental rhythm that guides perception and action. Neural oscillations create discrete processing states lasting approximately 30 milliseconds each. Events processed within a single state are experienced as simultaneous; only events spanning multiple states can be perceived as sequential. This rhythm appears across various measurements of brain activity, particularly in the gamma wave range around 40 Hz, corresponding to the temporal thresholds observed in behavioral studies. Evidence for these rhythmic mechanisms comes from multiple sources. The film industry inadvertently confirms this principle by using frame rates of at least 24 frames per second (about 42 milliseconds per frame) to create the illusion of continuous motion. At slower rates, we would perceive individual images rather than fluid movement. Additionally, patients with certain brain injuries, particularly to areas in the left temporal and parietal lobes, show elevated temporal order thresholds, suggesting these regions play key roles in temporal processing. Perhaps most intriguing are rare neurological cases where patients report experiencing a "time-lapse" phenomenon - everything around them seems to move too quickly, as though the world were on fast-forward. This might represent a slowing of their internal neural rhythm, causing external events to appear accelerated by comparison. Conversely, in dangerous situations, many people report experiencing a slow-motion effect, possibly because heightened arousal accelerates neural processing, making external events seem slower in relative terms. Researchers led by David Eagleman attempted to test this slow-motion phenomenon experimentally by having subjects view rapidly alternating images while free-falling from a tall platform. Though subjects reported subjective time dilation during the fall, their perceptual thresholds didn't improve, suggesting the slow-motion sensation might be a memory illusion rather than enhanced processing. However, the search continues for definitive evidence of how brain rhythms influence our subjective experience of time's flow.

Chapter 4: Present Moments and Mindfulness

The present moment has a special status in human experience. While we constantly anticipate the future and recall the past, we can only ever truly experience the now. But what exactly constitutes this "now"? How long does a moment last, and how can we cultivate greater presence within it? Research suggests that our experience of the present moment has a natural duration of approximately three seconds. Ernst Pöppel's analyses found this three-second window appearing consistently across cultures in music, poetry, and everyday behaviors. For example, musical motifs like the famous opening of Beethoven's Fifth Symphony, verses in poems, and conversational exchanges typically last about three seconds. This appears to reflect a fundamental integration mechanism in the brain that bundles perceptual information into coherent units of experience. We can observe this temporal integration in simple experiments. When listening to a metronome's evenly spaced beats, we automatically group them into patterns (tick-tock, tick-tock) rather than perceiving each beat in isolation. This grouping capability has limits - when beats occur more than three seconds apart, we experience them as separate events rather than a unified pattern. Similarly, when viewing ambiguous figures like the Necker cube or the rabbit-duck illusion, our perception typically switches between interpretations every two to three seconds. Mindfulness practices explicitly focus on enhancing our experience of the present moment. Jon Kabat-Zinn, who developed Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction, describes mindfulness as paying attention to the present in a particular way: on purpose, non-judgmentally, and with curiosity. This seemingly simple practice of directing attention to current experience - noticing bodily sensations, sounds, sights, thoughts - proves surprisingly difficult. Our minds naturally wander to past memories or future plans, pulling us away from the present. The benefits of cultivating presence extend beyond subjective experience. Research demonstrates that mindfulness training reduces pain, alleviates stress, diminishes depression symptoms, and improves cognitive function. Brain imaging studies show measurable changes in brain structure and activity following regular mindfulness practice, particularly in regions associated with attention, emotion regulation, and bodily awareness. Interestingly, breathing serves as a natural anchor for present-moment awareness. A typical relaxed breath lasts about three seconds - matching the brain's natural integration window for conscious experience. By focusing on the breath, as recommended in many meditation traditions, we synchronize with our inherent temporal rhythm, enhancing our capacity to experience the present moment fully. This alignment may help explain why breath-focused meditation has been practiced across diverse cultures for thousands of years as a pathway to greater presence and awareness.

Chapter 5: Body Time: How Our Physiology Shapes Time Perception

While we don't have a dedicated organ for sensing time like we do for vision or hearing, mounting evidence suggests our bodies serve as internal clocks. Our perception of time is intimately linked to bodily processes and states, creating what might be called "body time." The most well-established biological timekeeper is our circadian rhythm - the approximately 24-hour cycle regulating sleep, hormone production, body temperature, and other physiological functions. This internal clock synchronizes with environmental cues, particularly light, through a small region in the hypothalamus called the suprachiasmatic nucleus. When this synchronization is disrupted, as during jet lag or shift work, we experience profound disturbances in both physical functioning and time perception. People vary considerably in their circadian patterns, falling along a spectrum from "larks" (early risers) to "owls" (night owls). These chronotypes reflect genuine biological differences, not merely preferences or habits. Adolescents are naturally inclined toward later chronotypes for developmental reasons, yet most schools begin early in the morning. This mismatch creates what chronobiologist Till Roenneberg calls "social jetlag" - a chronic desynchronization between internal biological time and external social demands, contributing to sleep deprivation, reduced academic performance, and increased substance use among teenagers. Beyond circadian rhythms, research reveals connections between bodily signals and our perception of shorter time intervals. Studies conducted by Karin Meissner showed that heart rate patterns correlate with accuracy in time estimation tasks. As participants listened to tones of varying durations, their heart rates typically decreased, reaching minimum levels just as the tones ended. Remarkably, individuals with greater awareness of their heartbeats demonstrated more precise temporal judgments, suggesting interoceptive awareness (perception of internal bodily states) directly influences time perception. Emotional states powerfully affect time perception through bodily arousal. Fear, excitement, and stress typically cause time to feel expanded - a forty-five second encounter with a spider feels vastly longer to someone with arachnophobia than to someone without this fear. Conversely, pleasurable states of "flow" make time seem to vanish altogether. These emotional effects on time perception appear linked to changes in physiological arousal and bodily awareness. Brain imaging studies provide further evidence for this body-time connection. Research by Marc Wittmann and colleagues found that activity in the insular cortex - a brain region crucial for processing bodily signals - increases progressively during time estimation tasks, peaking when the estimated interval ends. This suggests the brain tracks time by accumulating bodily signals over the relevant period, creating our subjective sense of duration through interoceptive processes rather than through a specialized timing mechanism.

Chapter 6: Accelerating Life: Time as We Age

One of the most universal temporal experiences is the feeling that time speeds up as we age. Childhood summers seemed to stretch endlessly, while years fly by with increasing rapidity in adulthood. This phenomenon has been verified through multiple large-scale studies across different countries and cultures. But why does this acceleration occur? A common explanation involves the relative proportion of time relative to age - a year represents one-tenth of a ten-year-old's life but just one-eightieth of an eighty-year-old's existence. While mathematically elegant, this explanation doesn't fully account for our subjective experience. More compelling is the relationship between memory, novelty, and perceived duration. Our sense of how long a period lasted depends largely on how many distinct memories we form during that time. Childhood and adolescence overflow with novel experiences and firsts - first day of school, first kiss, first independent travel. Each new experience is emotionally charged and deeply encoded in memory. As we age, we encounter fewer truly novel situations. Life becomes more routine, with each workday resembling the last. Since these repetitive experiences require less cognitive processing, they leave fewer distinct memory traces. When we later reflect on these periods, the relative emptiness of memory makes them seem to have passed quickly. Thomas Mann captured this relationship between novelty and subjective time brilliantly in "The Magic Mountain," describing how the first days in a new place seem to stretch out due to the freshness of experience, while later days accelerate as habituation sets in. Israeli researchers empirically confirmed this pattern by interviewing vacationers, who consistently reported that their holidays seemed to pass more quickly as days progressed. This phenomenon creates what researchers call the "time paradox." When we're bored or waiting impatiently, time passes agonizingly slowly in the moment (prospective judgment). However, looking back later, these uneventful periods seem brief in memory (retrospective judgment). Conversely, engaging activities pass quickly in the moment but expand in retrospective memory due to their richness of experience. The implications for living a subjectively longer, fuller life are profound. By continually seeking novel experiences and breaking routines, we can potentially slow the subjective acceleration of time. Travel, learning new skills, meeting different people, and engaging in varied activities all create more distinctive memories, expanding our subjective timeline. However, novelty isn't the only path to meaningful temporal experience - depth matters too. The emotional richness developed in long-term relationships and vocations can create equally vivid memories. Our perception of time's acceleration also relates to our future perspective. As we age, we typically become more aware of life's finite nature. Interestingly, studies show that older adults don't necessarily focus less on the future than younger people - they simply shorten the timeframe of their planning, focusing on nearer-future goals rather than distant ones. This compressed future perspective may contribute to the feeling that time is passing more rapidly.

Chapter 7: Self-Consciousness and Temporal Experience

Self-consciousness and temporal experience are intimately connected. To be conscious is to perceive oneself existing in time - having a past, experiencing the present, and anticipating a future. This temporal dimension is not merely an add-on to consciousness but fundamental to its nature. Philosophers have long struggled with the paradox of self-consciousness. When I observe myself, I become the object of my own attention, yet the observing "I" remains elusive. As the Scottish philosopher David Hume noted, among all his perceptions, he could never find this "I" itself. Modern philosophers suggest we should speak not of the "subject" of experience but of the "subjectivity" of experience - consciousness is not a thing but a process of awareness that includes temporality as an essential aspect. Neuroscientists are now identifying brain mechanisms that might generate this temporal self-awareness. Antonio Damasio proposes that core consciousness arises from neural mapping of bodily states. Our emotions, which strongly color our experience of self, fundamentally involve feedback about physical conditions ranging from excitement to relaxation. Building on this work, A.D. (Bud) Craig highlights the insular cortex as crucial for integrating bodily signals with cognitive and emotional processes to create what he calls the "global emotional moment" - our sense of being a feeling subject in the present. This integration occurs through stages, with bodily signals arriving first in the posterior insular cortex and then progressing to the anterior regions, where they combine with information from other brain areas about social circumstances, thoughts, and motivations. The result is our subjective feeling of presence - experiencing ourselves as embodied beings persisting through time. Supporting this theory, researchers have identified specialized brain cells called von Economo neurons, particularly abundant in the anterior insular cortex, that appear only in mammals capable of self-awareness. Our experience of time and self becomes particularly pronounced in certain states. Boredom represents an extreme case - time passes painfully slowly, and we become uncomfortably aware of ourselves with nothing to occupy our attention. Conversely, in states of "flow" or during periods of intense activity, self-awareness and time perception recede, creating the paradoxical sensation that time has vanished. Modern society's acceleration brings unique challenges to temporal self-experience. The philosopher Martin Heidegger noted that "having no time" often correlates with losing oneself in activity - being so busy that deeper self-awareness becomes impossible. Sociologist Hartmut Rosa argues that technological acceleration, accelerating life pace, and the simultaneity of activities create a form of alienation from self and experience. We do more and more things simultaneously but experience each less deeply. Mindfulness practices offer a counterbalance to this acceleration, training attention to remain in the present moment and fostering deeper awareness of bodily states and temporal experience. By strengthening our capacity to direct attention and accept present circumstances, we can regain a sense of control over the pace of life. This doesn't mean withdrawing from society's acceleration but rather developing emotional intelligence to navigate it - choosing when to engage fully and when to disconnect, ultimately creating a more balanced relationship with time and self.

Summary

The way we experience time fundamentally shapes our existence, yet remains one of the most elusive aspects of human consciousness. Through this exploration of temporal perception, we've discovered that our sense of time emerges from the intricate interplay between brain rhythms, bodily processes, emotional states, and cultural contexts. Rather than possessing a dedicated "time organ," we construct our temporal experience through the integration of bodily signals and memories, creating a subjective timeline that can expand or contract depending on novelty, attention, and physiological states. Perhaps the most valuable insight is that we have more agency over our temporal experience than we might assume. By cultivating mindful awareness of the present moment, seeking novel experiences throughout life, and developing emotional intelligence about our relationship with time, we can potentially live richer, fuller lives regardless of chronological duration. The challenge in our accelerating world is not merely to have more time, but to experience time more fully - to be present in our bodies, attentive to our surroundings, and conscious of our existence as temporal beings. Future research may reveal even deeper connections between time perception, consciousness, and wellbeing, potentially transforming how we structure education, work, and social life to better align with our natural temporal rhythms.

Best Quote

“At home, with work over and done, what reason is there...not to spend an hour, every evening, doing x? X might stand for whatever holds personal interest: reading..., spending time on a collection of interesting objects, taking a nighttime stroll on one's own or with one's partner. Do one thing, with full attention, for an hour a day. Or just spend fifteen minutes sitting on the sofa and don't do anything for a change: How's my posture? Does anything hurt? How do I feel? We can give free rein to fantasy and observe how we may come to terms with the acceleration of the world that we experience.” ― Marc Wittmann, Felt Time: The Psychology of How We Perceive Time

Review Summary

Strengths: The book presents thought-provoking ideas and explores intriguing questions about human perception of time, subjective time experience, and the impact of experiences on time perception.\nWeaknesses: The writing style is criticized as "horrible," resembling a dissertation rather than a cohesive book. The book lacks a clear thesis or conclusion, and the presentation of topics such as Freud and psychoanalysis lacks analysis.\nOverall Sentiment: Mixed\nKey Takeaway: While the book offers engaging concepts and questions about time perception, its effectiveness is undermined by poor writing style and a lack of coherent structure and analysis.

About Author

Loading...
Marc Wittmann Avatar

Marc Wittmann

Marc Wittmann is Research Fellow at the Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health in Freiburg, Germany, and the author of Felt Time: The Psychology of How We Perceive Time (MIT Press).

Read more

Download PDF & EPUB

To save this Black List summary for later, download the free PDF and EPUB. You can print it out, or read offline at your convenience.

Book Cover

Felt Time

By Marc Wittmann

0:00/0:00

Build Your Library

Select titles that spark your interest. We'll find bite-sized summaries you'll love.