Home/Nonfiction/Suggestible You
Loading...
Suggestible You cover

Suggestible You

The Curious Science of Your Brain's Ability to Deceive, Transform, and Heal

4.0 (1,317 ratings)
23 minutes read | Text | 8 key ideas
Ever wondered if your mind holds the key to healing? In "Suggestible You," journalist Erik Vance unveils the astonishing science of suggestibility, weaving through a tapestry of placebos, hypnosis, and neurological wonders. This isn’t mere conjecture—Vance delves into centuries-old research and modern-day breakthroughs to reveal how our thoughts and beliefs can manifest real changes in our bodies. Journey from the hallowed halls of Harvard to the mystical practices of Catemaco’s shamans and Beijing’s alternative medicine, where the mind's power over matter takes center stage. With vivid storytelling and expert insights, this book challenges the boundaries of medicine and the mind, showing how the brain’s chemical magic can transform pain and perception. Dive into a world where your expectations might just be the most potent medicine of all.

Categories

Nonfiction, Self Help, Psychology, Health, Science, Audiobook, Medicine, Book Club, Neuroscience, Brain

Content Type

Book

Binding

Hardcover

Year

2016

Publisher

National Geographic

Language

English

File Download

PDF | EPUB

Suggestible You Plot Summary

Introduction

Your brain is constantly trying to predict what will happen next. It's an expectation machine, analyzing everything around you and making educated guesses about what's coming. This predictive power has kept humans alive for millennia, helping us avoid dangers and seize opportunities. But there's something extraordinary about this process: when your expectations are strong enough, they can actually change your reality. Consider what happens when you take a pain reliever for a headache. You might feel better the moment you swallow the pill, even though the medication takes 15-20 minutes to work. That immediate relief isn't from the drug—it's from your expectation that the pill will help. This remarkable phenomenon, known as the placebo effect, is just one example of how our minds can trick our bodies into feeling better, moving differently, or even healing faster. From the pain-killing powers of sugar pills to the healing suggestions of hypnosis, from false memories we swear are true to nocebo effects that make us feel worse, our suggestible minds shape our experiences in profound ways. By understanding these mechanisms, we gain insight not just into quirks of human psychology, but into powerful tools that can transform our health, performance, and perception of the world around us.

Chapter 1: The Power of Placebos: Storytelling and Modern Medicine

The word "placebo" comes from Latin, meaning "I shall please." In medicine, it typically refers to an inert substance like a sugar pill that produces a beneficial effect not because of any physical properties, but because the person taking it believes it will help. For thousands of years, much of medicine's power came from this effect, though doctors didn't fully understand the mechanism. From medieval treatments involving bird droppings and mercury to modern homeopathic remedies, many historical "cures" worked primarily through suggestion and expectation. The scientific study of placebos began in earnest during World War II with Dr. Henry Beecher, who noticed something puzzling while treating wounded soldiers. Many men with severe battlefield injuries refused painkillers despite their wounds, yet would complain about the minor pain of an injection. Beecher wondered why their perception of pain seemed so disconnected from the severity of their injuries. This observation led him to publish a groundbreaking 1955 paper called "The Powerful Placebo," which estimated that about 30% of patients across all medicine experience placebo responses to treatments. This revelation dramatically changed modern medicine. In 1962, following the thalidomide disaster (where a morning sickness drug caused severe birth defects), new laws required pharmaceuticals to prove their effectiveness beyond placebo effects before reaching the market. Suddenly, drug companies had to show their products weren't just safe but also more effective than sugar pills. This requirement revolutionized medicine but presented a major challenge: for conditions like pain, depression, and anxiety, placebo response rates can reach 80% or higher, making it difficult to prove a drug's independent value. What makes placebos so powerful? The key lies in storytelling and expectation. When a treatment comes with a compelling narrative that resonates with a patient's beliefs and experiences, it can unlock the brain's internal pharmacy. This is why more expensive placebos work better than cheap ones, why injections outperform pills, and why treatments delivered with confidence and care produce stronger effects. The placebo effect isn't just psychological trickery—it's a measurable biological phenomenon involving real changes in brain chemistry. When you expect pain relief, your brain actually produces its own painkillers, particularly endorphins, which are chemically similar to opiates like morphine. More recent research has revealed that placebos can be enhanced by social factors. If you believe others have experienced relief from a treatment, your own relief will likely be stronger. This explains why traditional medicines with long histories and cultural significance often produce powerful effects, regardless of their chemical properties. In a very real sense, the confidence of millions can become a medicine of its own.

Chapter 2: Inside Your Brain's Inner Pharmacy

What exactly happens in your brain during a placebo response? Researchers have discovered that when you expect relief from pain or other symptoms, your brain can release its own natural drugs. The most famous of these are endorphins—your body's homemade version of opioids like morphine—which were discovered in the 1970s. Scientists proved their role in the placebo effect through a clever experiment: they gave patients a placebo along with naloxone, a drug that blocks opioid receptors. When naloxone was administered, the placebo pain relief vanished, proving that natural opioids were responsible for the effect. Your internal pharmacy doesn't stop at endorphins. Dopamine, often called the "reward chemical," plays a crucial role in placebo responses, especially for conditions like Parkinson's disease. In one remarkable study, a Parkinson's patient in a clinical trial received what he thought was a cutting-edge medication. After taking it, he went from being wheelchair-bound to practically sprinting up stairs—only to learn later that he'd received a placebo. Brain scans showed his brain had released substantial amounts of dopamine, the very chemical his disease had depleted. Other chemicals in your inner pharmacy include serotonin (important for mood and digestion), endocannabinoids (similar to compounds in marijuana), and various hormones that regulate stress and inflammatory responses. The placebo effect operates through multiple pathways in the brain. When researchers use brain imaging to observe people experiencing placebo relief, they see activity begin in the prefrontal cortex—the advanced, logical part of the brain that processes expectations—and then work backward to more primitive regions that control pain and sensation. This suggests a collision of information: pain signals coming up from the body meet expectation signals coming down from the higher brain, and whatever emerges from this meeting is what you actually feel. Interestingly, not all conditions respond equally to placebos. They work best for subjective symptoms that involve the nervous system and brain chemicals—pain, depression, anxiety, nausea, fatigue, and some aspects of Parkinson's disease. They're less effective for objective measures like tumor size, blood sugar levels, or bacterial infections. This pattern makes sense when you consider which bodily systems are most directly influenced by the chemicals in your internal pharmacy. Your susceptibility to placebos may be partially determined by your genetics. Researchers have discovered that variations in genes like COMT, which helps regulate dopamine levels in your brain, can predict how strongly you'll respond to placebos. People with a particular version of this gene that leaves more dopamine floating around in their brains appear to be more placebo-responsive. However, placebo responses aren't fixed traits—they can vary based on context, past experiences, cultural beliefs, and even how much you trust your doctor. Rather than seeing placebo responders as gullible or weak-minded, scientists now recognize that a strong placebo response represents a valuable skill—the ability to harness your brain's natural healing capacities. Those who respond strongly to placebos aren't being fooled; they're tapping into a powerful internal system that many of us access only partially or not at all.

Chapter 3: The Search for Placebo Responders

For decades, scientists and pharmaceutical companies have tried to identify what makes someone a "placebo responder." Is it personality? Intelligence? Gender? Age? The quest has proven surprisingly difficult. Early theories suggesting that placebo responders were more gullible, less educated, or more neurotic have all failed to hold up under scientific scrutiny. Even attempts to screen out placebo responders at the beginning of drug trials don't work—new placebo responders simply emerge as the study continues. The difficulty in identifying placebo responders creates serious challenges for drug development. When testing new medications for conditions like pain or depression, pharmaceutical companies must prove their products outperform placebos—a high bar when placebo response rates can reach 80% or higher. This requirement has become so challenging that many companies have abandoned research into treatments for certain conditions, finding it too expensive and risky to develop drugs that might fail against the powerful placebo effect. A potential breakthrough came in 2012 when researcher Kathryn Hall discovered a genetic component to placebo response. She focused on the COMT gene, which produces an enzyme that regulates dopamine levels in the brain. This gene comes in three variations: some people have two copies of a version that efficiently cleans up dopamine (val/val), others have two copies that work less efficiently (met/met), leaving more dopamine available, and about half the population has one of each (val/met). In a study of irritable bowel syndrome patients receiving placebo acupuncture, those with the met/met variant showed dramatically stronger responses, especially when treated by warm, caring practitioners. This genetic discovery suggests that placebo responsiveness isn't a weakness but a form of neurological talent—the ability to self-heal through expectation. It also explains why some people seem to benefit tremendously from alternative therapies while others experience nothing. Rather than dismissing alternative medicine entirely, we might recognize that for people with certain genetic profiles, these treatments effectively trigger internal healing mechanisms. The challenge is determining who belongs to which group. The genetics of placebo response may eventually transform drug development and treatment approaches. Some companies are already exploring ways to screen trial participants based on their genetic profiles, potentially allowing more targeted testing of new medications. In the future, doctors might test your genetic makeup to determine whether you'd benefit more from conventional pharmaceuticals or from treatments that leverage your natural placebo response. A powerful example of how expectation can shape reality comes from Mike Pauletich, a man diagnosed with Parkinson's disease who participated in a gene therapy trial. After receiving what he believed was an experimental treatment, his symptoms dramatically improved—he went from struggling with basic movements to running triathlons and skiing. Years later, when the trial results were published, he discovered he had received the placebo. Remarkably, three years after this revelation, his improvements continued. His case suggests that under the right circumstances, the brain's response to expectation can create lasting, even permanent changes to neurological function.

Chapter 4: Nocebos: When Suggestion Harms

While placebos represent the healing power of positive expectations, nocebos reveal the darker side of this same mechanism. The term "nocebo" (Latin for "I shall harm") describes negative outcomes produced by negative expectations. If believing a sugar pill will help you can make you feel better, believing it will harm you can make you feel worse—sometimes dramatically so. Nocebo effects follow similar neurological pathways as placebos but engage different brain chemicals. Where placebos often trigger endorphins, nocebos activate stress hormones and a substance called cholecystokinin (CCK), which intensifies pain perception. Fabrizio Benedetti, a pioneer in nocebo research, demonstrated this by giving patients saline injections while telling them it would increase their pain. Not only did their pain worsen, but when he blocked CCK with another drug, the nocebo effect disappeared—proving that specific brain chemicals mediate these negative responses. Nocebo effects are surprisingly common in everyday life. When patients experience side effects from medications, a significant percentage of those effects may actually be nocebos. In clinical trials, people receiving sugar pills instead of active drugs regularly report headaches, nausea, dizziness, and other symptoms that match the side effects they were warned about. Even more striking, patients in placebo groups sometimes experience more side effects than those receiving the actual medication. Fear makes nocebos particularly powerful—more powerful than placebos in many cases. From an evolutionary perspective, this makes sense. If our ancestors were overly optimistic about dangerous situations, they might take fatal risks. But if they were overly cautious about potential threats, they would survive, even if occasionally inconvenienced. This biological bias toward negative expectations explains why health scares spread so quickly and why conditions like "wind turbine syndrome" (symptoms attributed to living near wind farms) can affect entire communities despite lacking scientific basis. Nocebo effects may contribute to some mysterious modern ailments. When people develop symptoms after exposure to electromagnetic fields, chemicals, or foods they believe are harmful—despite scientific evidence suggesting these exposures are safe—nocebo mechanisms may be at work. This doesn't mean the symptoms aren't real; the pain, fatigue, or nausea is genuinely felt. But the trigger may be expectation rather than physical harm. Perhaps the most extreme manifestation of nocebo effects is found in cases of "voodoo death," where people who believe they've been cursed actually die from the fear itself. Anthropologist Walter Cannon documented cases where otherwise healthy individuals wasted away after being told they were under a spell. Modern explanations suggest extreme stress responses can trigger cardiovascular collapse or suppress immune function. While rare, these cases demonstrate the profound power negative expectations can have over bodily functions. Understanding nocebos has important implications for medical practice. Simple changes in how doctors describe procedures—saying "you'll feel a cool sensation" instead of "this will sting"—can significantly reduce pain. Similarly, how medication side effects are explained can influence whether patients experience them. The challenge is balancing honest informed consent with avoiding unnecessary nocebo responses.

Chapter 5: Hypnosis and the Trance-like State

Hypnosis represents one of the most direct ways to harness the power of suggestion. While often portrayed in entertainment as a form of mind control, real hypnosis is better understood as a state of focused attention and heightened suggestibility. During hypnosis, people remain conscious and in control, but their minds become more receptive to certain suggestions, particularly those involving sensory perception and bodily sensations. The practice has ancient roots, appearing in various forms across cultures, but scientific investigation began in the late 1700s with Franz Mesmer, who believed he could manipulate an invisible "magnetic fluid" in patients' bodies. Though Mesmer's theories were discredited (by a commission including Benjamin Franklin), the trance-like states he induced were real. By the mid-1800s, physicians were using hypnosis for pain control during surgeries—before chemical anesthesia was available. Despite this legitimate medical history, hypnosis developed a dubious reputation due to its associations with stage entertainment, spiritualism, and unsubstantiated claims. Modern research has revealed that hypnosis produces measurable changes in brain activity. During hypnotic states, electroencephalogram (EEG) readings show increased theta waves, which are slower than normal waking brain rhythms but faster than sleep patterns. Brain scans reveal altered activity in regions involving attention, pain perception, and sensory processing. Importantly, people differ dramatically in how susceptible they are to hypnosis—about 10% of the population is highly hypnotizable, another 10% seems completely resistant, and the rest fall somewhere in between. This variability in hypnotic susceptibility appears to be a stable trait, similar to IQ, that doesn't change much throughout life. Contrary to popular belief, being easily hypnotized isn't linked to gullibility or low intelligence. Instead, researchers now view hypnotizability as a form of talent—the ability to focus deeply and engage imaginatively with suggested scenarios. People who are highly hypnotizable often score high on measures of "absorption," the capacity to become completely engrossed in experiences like reading, music, or natural beauty. Hypnosis has proven especially effective for pain management. At the University of Washington, researcher David Patterson has used hypnosis to help burn patients endure excruciating wound treatments without medication. In controlled studies, hypnosis significantly reduces pain perception and anxiety during medical procedures. It's also shown effectiveness for conditions like irritable bowel syndrome, smoking cessation, and certain phobias. The mechanism appears to involve altering how the brain processes sensory information rather than simply distracting from it. Interestingly, hypnosis and placebos, while similar in some ways, operate through different neural pathways. Drugs that block placebo effects don't block hypnotic responses, suggesting distinct mechanisms. Hypnosis also seems to create deeper changes in perception than standard placebos. Where a placebo might dull pain, hypnosis can sometimes eliminate it entirely. Another key difference is that hypnosis works best for people who are naturally susceptible to it, while placebo responses can potentially be triggered in almost anyone under the right circumstances. As with many aspects of suggestibility, researchers have worked to identify what makes someone hypnotizable based on personality or genetic factors, but clear patterns remain elusive. What's becoming increasingly clear, however, is that this natural variation in suggestibility represents an important aspect of human diversity—one that could potentially be harnessed for personalized approaches to healthcare, pain management, and psychological treatment.

Chapter 6: False Memories: Rewriting Your Past

Our memories feel like faithful recordings of past events—complete with sights, sounds, and emotions—but science tells us they're actually reconstructions created anew each time we recall them. This reconstructive nature makes memory surprisingly vulnerable to suggestion and distortion, allowing entirely false memories to form with the same vividness and emotional impact as real ones. The creation of memory occurs in three stages: encoding (when we first experience an event), consolidation (the process of transferring information to long-term storage), and retrieval (when we access the memory later). Errors can occur at any stage, but false memories typically involve problems during retrieval, when our minds fill in missing details or incorporate new information into existing memories. Unlike simply forgetting something, with false memories, we don't realize any error has occurred—we feel certain our recollection is accurate. Elizabeth Loftus, a pioneering researcher in this field, has demonstrated how easily false memories can be created. In one famous series of experiments, she showed participants footage of a car accident, then asked questions using different verbs to describe the collision—"smashed," "hit," or "contacted." Those who heard "smashed" consistently estimated higher speeds and were more likely to report seeing broken glass that wasn't actually present. In more dramatic studies, Loftus successfully implanted entirely fictional childhood memories in about 25% of participants—convincing them they had been lost in a shopping mall as children or had met Bugs Bunny at Disneyland (an impossible memory since Bugs Bunny is a Warner Bros. character, not Disney). False memories became a matter of significant public concern in the 1980s and 1990s during what became known as the "Satanic Panic." Numerous cases emerged where people, often under hypnosis or suggestive therapy, "recovered" memories of childhood ritual abuse that investigators later determined were highly unlikely to have occurred. In one town in Florida, dozens of children came to believe they had been victims of satanic rituals at their preschool, leading to criminal charges and community hysteria. Years later, many of these individuals realized their "memories" had been inadvertently created through suggestive questioning and social pressure. The science behind false memories reveals several patterns. They form more easily when they align with existing beliefs or expectations—it's easier to accept a false memory that fits your self-image or worldview. Emotional intensity also plays a role; highly emotional false memories can feel more real than mundane true ones. And when memories are repeatedly retrieved and discussed, they become more susceptible to distortion with each retelling, often becoming more dramatic or more aligned with a coherent narrative over time. Brain imaging studies have attempted to distinguish true from false memories, but so far without clear success. Activity patterns look remarkably similar whether someone is recalling a genuine experience or one that never happened. This neurological similarity helps explain why false memories feel so authentic to the person experiencing them—the brain processes them using the same mechanisms as real memories. Understanding false memory has profound implications for legal proceedings that rely on eyewitness testimony. Numerous cases of wrongful conviction have occurred when witnesses confidently identified suspects who were later exonerated by DNA evidence. Police interview techniques, lineup procedures, and even the wording of questions can dramatically influence what witnesses "remember." In response, many jurisdictions have adopted new practices like double-blind lineups (where neither the witness nor the administering officer knows which person is the suspect) and videotaped interviews to reduce memory contamination. False memories aren't necessarily pathological—they're a by-product of the same flexible, associative memory system that allows us to learn from experience and predict future events. Our brains evolved to extract patterns and meaning rather than to record perfect detail. This flexibility usually serves us well, but it also makes us vulnerable to suggestion and misinformation, reminding us that even our most vivid recollections should sometimes be approached with healthy skepticism.

Summary

At its core, the suggestible mind reveals a profound truth about human experience: what we expect often becomes our reality. Our brains are not passive recorders of the world but active prediction machines, constantly shaping what we perceive based on what we believe will happen. This mechanism—seen in placebos that ease pain, nocebos that create suffering, hypnotic suggestions that alter perception, and false memories that rewrite our past—represents not a flaw but a fundamental feature of human consciousness. It explains how sugar pills can trigger real healing, how negative expectations can manifest as physical symptoms, and how our memories reshape themselves to fit the stories we tell about our lives. The implications of this knowledge extend far beyond understanding medical curiosities or psychological oddities. By recognizing the power of expectation, we gain potential tools for enhancing health, performance, and wellbeing. Perhaps instead of seeing suggestibility as gullibility, we might recognize it as a valuable skill—the ability to harness the brain's internal pharmacy. What other aspects of our lives might be transformed by deliberate shifts in expectation? How might healthcare change if doctors leveraged these mechanisms more intentionally? And what might we achieve if we learned to direct our expectations toward our goals rather than our fears? The suggestible mind invites us to reconsider not just how we think about health and medicine, but how we understand ourselves and the reality we experience every day.

Best Quote

“Humans are not alone in creating false memories; pigeons, mice, and even bumblebees seem to have them.c That suggests false memories may just be a part of how we think—a by-product of our innate ability to group things together by theme. As Schacter says, memories are a tool to help animals predict the future by using the past. And to do that, we humans have become experts at quickly grouping things together and creating patterns.” ― Erik Vance, Suggestible You: The Curious Science of Your Brain's Ability to Deceive, Transform, and Heal

Review Summary

Strengths: "Engaging storytelling and intriguing case studies illustrate the power of the mind effectively. Vance's ability to connect scientific insights with real-world implications stands out. A significant positive is the book's thorough research, making complex scientific concepts accessible to a general audience. The exploration of cultural and individual differences in suggestibility provides particularly insightful perspectives." Weaknesses: "Occasional meandering in structure can detract from the book's focus. While compelling, the anecdotes sometimes overshadow the scientific explanations." Overall Sentiment: "The book is generally well-received, offering an enlightening read that appeals to those interested in psychology and neuroscience. Its blend of scientific research and personal anecdotes engages a wide audience." Key Takeaway: "The book emphasizes the profound impact of suggestibility on human perception and belief, highlighting how beliefs and expectations can shape experiences across various aspects of life."

About Author

Loading...
Erik Vance Avatar

Erik Vance

Read more

Download PDF & EPUB

To save this Black List summary for later, download the free PDF and EPUB. You can print it out, or read offline at your convenience.

Book Cover

Suggestible You

By Erik Vance

0:00/0:00

Build Your Library

Select titles that spark your interest. We'll find bite-sized summaries you'll love.